Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff. Группа авторов

Читать онлайн книгу.

Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
behavior problems than owners of cats weaned in the 12–15‐week range. Kittens weaned before 8 weeks were more likely to show aggressive behaviors than those weaned later. Later weaning was a protective factor against aggression toward other cats as well as familiar and unfamiliar people. The prevalence of abnormal oral behaviors like excessive grooming and wool sucking as well as shyness toward novel objects decreased in the kittens weaned after 14 weeks as well (Ahola et al. 2017; Houpt 2018). Overall, owners of cats weaned before 8 weeks of age were more likely to report behavior problems than those weaned in the 12–13‐week range. Aggression, abnormal oral behaviors, and shyness toward novel objects were shown to be inversely correlated to the age of weaning as well (Ahola et al. 2017).

      2.5.2.5 Adult (Sexual and Social Maturity)

      The juvenile period ends with sexual maturity. In female cats, this correlates with the first sign of estrus, which can be as early as 3 to 4 months and as late as 12 months of age. Environmental factors such as the time of year born, exposure to mature tomcats, the presence of other female cats in estrus, and increasing periods of light all influence the age of estrus onset. Male domestic cats reach sexual maturity between 9 and 12 months. In free‐ranging cat colonies, however, a male may not become reproductively active until two or three years of age, when full integration into a colony is achieved (Hart and Hart 2014a). Social maturity is the stage of final transition into adult behaviors such as territoriality and aggression and in domestic cats is thought to occur between 36 and 48 months of age (Landsberg et al. 2013).

      2.6.1 Play

      Play is ubiquitous amongst many genera of animals, and cats are no exception. Domestic cat play behaviors are classified as social, locomotor, predatory, or object play (Delgado and Hecht 2019). It has been widely assumed that play is neuromuscular, social, and cognitive preparation for critical adult behaviors (Burghardt 2005). However, research of kittens raised in barren environments provided evidence that play does not seem to be a required precursor for many behaviors, particularly those like predation that are related to survival (Thomas and Schaller 1954). Early experiences can impact the timing and character of play behaviors. Genetics, sex, learning, and characteristics of the queen, litter, and target of play can alter the development of play behaviors in cats (Delgado and Hecht 2019).

Photo depicts kitten object play.

      Source: Reproduced with permission from M. Allison. © Meg Allison.

      2.6.2 Hunting

      Cats are highly motivated to hunt. In fact, hunger and recent feeding behavior may reduce consumption and time spent hunting but not the motivation to stalk and capture prey (Fitzgerald and Turner 2000). Cats may even stop eating if the opportunity to hunt and kill presents itself during a meal. A cat may eat 10 to 20 small meals throughout a 24‐hour period, although this may be concentrated to crepuscular periods in free‐roaming cats but spread throughout the day for housecats. In general, cats living in human homes tend to take on a more diurnal pattern like humans (Overall et al. 2005). Although hunting is a reflexive behavioral pattern displayed even if a cat is raised in isolation, maternal influence and early experience can shape hunting behavior and food preference (Kuo 1930, 1938). Cats are less likely to predate on small mammals if raised with these potential prey species from a young age, preferably starting during the two to seven weeks of age socialization period (Kuo 1938).

      2.6.3 Ingestive Behavior

      Cats are obligate carnivores and have specific dietary requirements best met through animal‐based diets. Several essential compounds cannot be autosynthesized by cats and, therefore, must be obtained from the environment. Animal products contain sulfur‐rich amino acids (methionine, cysteine, taurine), vitamins (niacin and thiamine), essential fatty acids, and high‐protein content necessary for basic feline metabolic functions. Moreover, cats are limited in their ability to process plant‐derived foods (Zoran and Buffington 2011).

      Cats can be picky eaters, developing a strong preference for only a few foods while refusing to eat others (Overall et al. 2005; Stasiak 2002). Food preference or aversion can be mitigated if food items with a variety of textures and flavors are presented to kittens at a young age. Cats may develop a learned aversion if a certain food becomes associated with nausea, force feeding, or medication administration (Stasiak 2001).

      Some cats display unacceptable or abnormal oral behaviors. Chewing or ingesting non‐food materials (pica) such as plants, fabric, plastic, rubber, cords, or string has been reported by some cat owners (Houpt 2018). Plant or grass eating seems to be a normal behavior, although anecdotally the practice has been linked to gastrointestinal purging (Hart and Hart 2014a). Discomfort due to dental or abdominal pain as well as any metabolic, organ, or neurologic disease should be ruled out or addressed as potential factors in abnormal oral behaviors. Hunger may trigger


Скачать книгу