The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов

Читать онлайн книгу.

The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
be visualized by gross inspection of brain tissue at post‐mortem or with neuroimaging. Cipolla (2009) estimates that the total length of capillaries within the human brain, if laid end‐on‐end is approximately 400 miles. The co‐development of cerebral vasculature is equally important because neural cells have no capacity to store glucose or oxygen, and myelin development requires delivery of critical nutrients to build and maintain the fatty sheath that coats the axon for neural transmission. Since each brain cell is dependent on the oxygen and nutrient exchange that occurs within this capillary bed, any disruption in vascular development or intactness also implies disrupted cellular integrity.

      The importance of the vasculature to supply glucose for brain development cannot be overstated. As pointed out by Steiner (2019, p. 8): “It has been estimated that during childhood the brain may account for up to 60% of the body basal energetic requirements.” Returning to Figure 3.1 and the proportional development of the brain in relation to the fetal/infant body is information enough to implicate the enormous requirements for energy, more‐so than any other body part. The cellular processes combined with nutritional demands to meet the energy needs of the growing brain are complex, essential for healthy brain growth and age‐typical social brain development (Laffel, 1999; McKenna et al., 2015).

      Historically, human developmental neuroscience research was entirely dependent on postmortem examination (Ernhart, 1991; Eskenazi et al., 1988; Towbin, 1978) and animal studies (Rakic, 1978). These early postmortem investigations often examined specific regions of interest (ROI) involving certain brain structures, often reporting size, type of cells, and cellular configurations, but such procedures were extremely time‐consuming, requiring meticulous dissection and effort (Blinkov & Glezer, 1968). Of course, since this was all postmortem, none of this could be related to social‐emotional functioning in the living child, unless an antemortem, anecdotal record had some information about social behavior. Despite these pre‐neuroimaging limitations, it was established that there were a minimum of four key features of brain growth that related to its size and assumed importance for social development: (1) myelination increased throughout childhood and adolescence, (2) changes in cellular density within GM occurred during development, which actually included apoptosis (see Figure 3.1) and pruning, (3) synaptic complexity increased along with neural connectivity, and (4) the development of integrated neural networks (Davison & Dobbing, 1966; Herschkowitz & Rossi, 1971).

      The ability to study these four areas and their relevance to social brain development, all changed with the introduction of computed tomography (CT) in the early 1970s, followed by magnetic resonance (MR) a few years later. These neuroimaging technologies permitted in vivo assessment of brain structure, providing the first direct visualization and quantitative metrics to investigate brain development. The initial problem was that CT involved radiation exposure, so not a brain imaging method possible for normative, and especially longitudinal studies of children, brain, and social development. Nonetheless, CT rapidly became instrumental in identifying various aspects of brain pathology in pediatric neurological and neuropsychiatric disease and acquired injuries, which in turn, permitted the study of the developing brain in the living child who had a change in social behavior (Bigler et al., 2013; Yeates et al., 2007). With CT, the first in vivo studies emerged showing how acquired lesions, especially from trauma, altered social‐emotional functioning in children (Bigler, 1999). Now with contemporary neuroimaging methods this approach to studying damage to the social brain network has become commonplace as reviewed by Ryan et al. (2021).

      Using this quantitative approach with MRI scans, both Courchesne et al. (2000) and Pfefferbaum et al. (1994) calculated total intracranial volume (TICV) and plotted that along with total brain volume (TBV), WM and GM volume. Growth curves of both TICV and TBV mirror one another, with correlations that exceed 0.90 (see Bigler, 2021) through ~ 5 years of age, the HC, TICV, and TBV measures remain highly intercorrelated.

      As shown in Figure 3.1, within GM, once a peak is reached there is a period of apoptosis (cell death) and cellular pruning. Theoretically, the initial excess of neural cells reflects a buffer against potential traumatic birth injury as well as dealing with inherent subtle errors making some cells expendable (Towbin, 1978). As cells compete for functional connectivity but do not become established or have an error in synaptic function, biologically these cells may not be essential. This competition from inclusion/exclusion in the neural developmental matrix that forms the brain, has been viewed as a type of a Darwinian competition for neural cell survival (Szilagyi et al., 2016). The apoptosis may come about because of critical periods where either input or output connections fail to sufficiently occur and the cell dies or is “pruned” back (Moreno et al., 2015).


Скачать книгу