The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов

Читать онлайн книгу.

The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
M. J. (2004, April). The case against perfection. The Atlantic Monthly.

      51 Shaffer, A., Suveg, C., Thomassin, K., & Bradbury, L. L. (2012). Emotion socialization in the context of family risks: Links to child emotion regulation. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21(6), 917–924.

      52 Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. Knopf.

      53 Vincent, C., & Ball, S. J. (2007). “Making up” the middle‐class child: Families, activities and class dispositions. Sociology, 41(6), 1061–1077.

      54 Whitebread, D. (2017). Free play and children’s mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 1(3), 167–169.

      55 Wyness, M. (2020). The responsible parent and networks of support: A case study of school engagement in a challenging environment. British Educational Research Journal, 46(1), 161–176.

      56 Young Women’s Trust (2017). What matters to young mums? Young Women’s Trust.

      57 Zahn‐Waxler, C., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Marceau, K. (2008). Disorders of childhood and adolescence: Gender and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 275–303.

       Heather Montgomery

      While rarely referred to as “social development” within anthropology the ways that children develop and learn about their worlds, how they interact with others and how, through these interactions, grow up to be competent and successful members of their societies and cultures, have been much studied by anthropologists. There is a long‐standing interest in socialization, particularly among American anthropologists, who have focused on how childrearing practices shape children’s identities and social development and, more recently, on how children learn the skills necessary to work and contribute to their communities. This work has developed in parallel with psychology, drawing heavily on the findings and insights from social psychology and contributing cross‐cultural perspectives and examples to discussions of how children learn and develop. There is an excellent summary and discussion of this work in LeVine (2007), and an invaluable reader with extracts from core texts in the field edited by LeVine and New (2008).

      In this chapter I will discuss these two different “anthropologies of childhood” and look at how they have both contributed to understandings of children’s social development – and problematized them. I will then look at the tensions between them, as well as areas of contemporary and future overlap, and at the benefits of combining multiple perspectives to create vibrant and holistic studies of childhood which move beyond disciplinary backgrounds.

      From the earliest days of the discipline, studies of child development were central to North American anthropologists. Franz Boas (1916), for example, used studies of child development to chart the environmental impact of human physiology among immigrants to America. His interest in children and young people also had a profound influence on his student, Margaret Mead, who became for many decades the most prominent figure in anthropological studies of the social and cultural development of children. It was with Boas’s encouragement that Margaret Mead began her studies of Samoa (1928) and New Guinea (1930), focusing her attention on children and young people, looking at how they were brought up, and the effects that their upbringing had on their adult personality and behavior. In her work she analyzed the daily lives of Samoan girls from infancy through early childhood, adolescence, and beyond, describing a stress‐free passage through life without the tensions and disruptions at particular life‐stages (especially adolescence) that Western psychologists claimed to be universal behaviors and developmental norms. Mead argued that any such behaviors were as much culturally learned as biologically innate and that patterns of socialization, rather than physical changes, were key to understanding children’s social development. Although Mead was subsequently heavily criticized for both her methodology and her interpretation (see Freeman, 1983), Coming of Age in Samoa (Mead 1928) has remained in print ever since and its popularity amongst both psychologists and anthropologists, as well as its explicitly comparative stance, continues to be influential and important in understanding the cross‐cultural differences in children’s development.

      Although Margaret Mead’s influence declined in American anthropology in the 1950s, studies of childrearing and socialization continued through the work of John Whiting and others. After fieldwork in Papua New Guinea, Whiting turned his attention to cross‐cultural research, focusing his interest on broader patterns of human behavior, and their links to childhood experiences. Using material from the Human Relations Area Files (HRAF), the immense database of ethnographic information set up in 1949 by George Murdock at Yale as a way of making statistical cross‐cultural comparisons, Whiting and his collaborators set out to undertake systematic analyses of childhood experiences, the impacts they had on their social development and the effects they then had on adult society.


Скачать книгу