The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов

Читать онлайн книгу.

The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
for each offspring produced. For example in mammals, where very large numbers are produced, such as in rabbits (up to 120 offspring each year per female), very little time and effort go into each offspring, which reach reproductive capability quite rapidly. In stark contrast, where small numbers are produced, such as in bonobos (one offspring every 4 years per female), then each has a lengthy period of development prior to reproduction. Where large numbers of offspring are produced with little investment in each, this situation is known as r selection, whereas where very few offspring are produced and each spends a lengthy period of development (and high parental investment) this is known as K selection.

      To complicate matters, these two extreme strategies are also known as fast (r) and slow (K) and rather than being two strategies, we should think of them as falling on either end of a spectrum (Rubenstein & Alcock, 2018). Interestingly, in the latter years of the 20th century it became apparent that individuals within a species can also make decisions about whether to concentrate on current (fast/r) or later (slow/K) reproductive effort. As we will see, as part of an LH approach, this fast/r to slow/K continuum has been integrated into our understanding of the relationship between attachment styles and later child (and adult) behavior.

      In 1997, Belsky developed this psychosocial acceleration theory further by proposing adaptationist accounts for each of Ainsworth’s three attachment styles. Belsky focused primarily on adult outcomes but he also considered how childhood behaviors can be related to these forms of attachment. In the case of secure attachments, children come to see the world as one in which relationships are rewarding and enduring. Such children, therefore, make lasting friendships during childhood and tend not to respond to social challenges with aggression and resentment. Where insecure‐avoidant attachments are formed, Belsky suggests resources may be limited and others cannot be trusted. Hence, both boys and girls find it difficult to form trusting relationships. Both sexes are also more likely to be opportunistic in how they treat other children (and later in adult relationships). For those children who experience insecure‐resistant attachments there is a tendency towards clinginess and demands for others’ attention. To Belsky such children are the least likely to go on to have children themselves but instead shift their investment to other relatives such as younger siblings or nephews and nieces. Belsky suggests in the case of insecure‐resistant attachments children develop a “helper‐at‐the‐nest” role and shift to an indirect reproductive role.

      According to modern day evolutionists, both forms of insecure attachment can be seen as alternate LH strategies to boost inclusive fitness under challenging conditions (which may have been common in the ancestral past, see for example, Del Giudice [2009a]). Encouragingly, Belsky’s approach to LH theory and psychosocial complexity is broadly compatible with the now widely accepted view, discussed earlier, that human brain size evolved in response to increased group size and social complexity.

Schematic illustration of developmental pathways for fast and slow life history strategies.

      Although Belsky and his co‐worker’s model was largely theoretical when originally introduced, today, as evolutionary developmental psychology has progressed, evidence to support specific aspects of it has accumulated. Exposure to stress or maltreatment early on in life, for example, is now known to be associated both with various forms of gene expression related to stress and with internalizing behaviors (behaviors such as feeling sad, lonely, and withdrawn) between the ages of 3 to 5 (Conradt et al., 2016; de Baca & Ellis, 2017; Romens et al., 2015).

      While the integration of LH theory into models of human development has helped to revolutionize the field, we should not think of LH strategies as becoming immutable due to early infant attachment experiences. Some evolutionary theorists believe that LH strategies are not prescribed at an early stage in development but rather show a degree of flexibility, based around a series of developmental switch point transitions. This notion of transitions in development is particularly associated with Marco Del Giudice. According to Del Giudice, during development there are a series of points when the LH strategy might be attenuated leading to changes in social behavior during childhood.

      It is


Скачать книгу