The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов
Читать онлайн книгу.of ability can later lead to improvements in ability. Myers and Bjorklund (2020) suggest there may be a whole raft of such social and cognitive ontogenetic adaptations that are appropriate for that age, but which then disappear.
Deferred adaptations are those which have no immediate benefit but are useful later in development. Social and object play are good examples of this. Many social skills are learned through engaging in play with others. Such skills are put to good use later in life in order to negotiate the social landscape. Many would suggest that infants and young children play together because it is enjoyable and leads to immediate gratification. But the ultimate reason why it is enjoyable, according to evolutionists such as Geary and Bjorklund, is because ancestral children who found it to be rewarding reaped the benefits later in life and passed on this proclivity to their offspring.
Finally, conditional adaptations can be seen as a special case of deferred adaptations in that they are environmentally conditional. This means that, given an unpredictable environment, it pays children to later be able to alter their developmental trajectory (up to a point: Barkow, 1989). In the case of sexual maturation, those under harsh conditions might leave more surviving offspring by developing to sexual maturity earlier. Note that this is part‐and‐parcel of Belsky’s conception of LH theory. In fact, all three of these types of developmental adaptations are conducive with an LH approach to understanding the relationship between evolution and development. Overall, it is fair to say that LH theory has provided a useful framework for understating the relationship between social development and evolutionary theory.
Present Concerns and Future Developments: Cultural Editing
An evolutionary stance that begins with the EEA helps us to look to the past to prepare for the present and the future. One problem that evolution helps us to understand is the ongoing dismay of parents all over the world (Barkow, 2014, Barkow et al., 2012): many ambitious adolescents equate “success” with being a successful entertainer, especially with being a rapper or rock star. Parents who for generations have been farmers or glassblowers or members of the clergy learn, at times with chagrin, of their children’s ambitions. Almost everywhere, many mothers and fathers ask the same question, “why won’t the children listen?” The answer has to do with cultural editing.
Very briefly, this parental pain is the result of the new mass media subverting the psychological mechanisms underlying cultural editing. Cultural editing is a process without which we could not have evolved the distinctive human trait of massive cultural transmission (Barkow, 1989, 2014; Barkow et al., 2012). Our main advantage over other animal species is that, due, in part, to our ability to share attention and intention and to cooperate, we are the most cultural of the world’s species, we have evolved to adapt to our environment by socially transmitting vast encyclopedias of frequently useful information. That information is passed on within generations and across generations, but this is a very risky process: environments alter, errors occur, and exemplars die without having disseminated the knowledge they carry. And so, information that once was likely adaptive may now be lost or neutral or maladaptive or simply mistaken (Barkow, 1989). We therefore have been selected for editing mechanisms.
One such evolved mechanism is paying automatic attention to and learning preferentially from the high in status, that is, the prestigious. Exactly what is prestigious may differ over time and from society to society, what does not change is that human societies are hierarchical in terms of respect/prestige structures and that both children and adults attend preferentially to, and learn from, the high in status. In earlier environments this was an effective mechanism because the high in status were more likely to be doing things right, adaptively, than the low in status. For example, it was adaptive to respect and therefore attend to and learn from farmers whose crops were always healthy or to women whose babies grew and prospered or to potters whose work did not crack in the kiln: at least some of what was learned would likely have been useful practices, while the errors that reduced the status of some were less likely to be transmitted. In the modern era, however, this simple mechanism is being subverted because the people at the top of the prestige ladder are likely to be entertainers or athletes or simply fictitious characters. There is nothing wrong with wanting to be an entertainer or athlete, but only a tiny proportion of “wannabes” can make it to the top because modern media mean that society does not require a vast number of individuals in these fields. Worse, there is an opportunity cost because the young people are failing to learn from parents and other relatives and neighbors and teachers who do have locally relevant knowledge and behaviors that would be helpful in achieving local respect and prestige. These local sources of cultural knowledge are often ignored because they are perceived as being of much lower status than the athletes and entertainer (and some politicians) and “influencers” presented by social media: the cultural editing mechanism “attend to and learn preferentially from the high in status” has been subverted, hijacked by screens, and the cultural knowledge transmission process compromised.
The cultural editing framework leads to many questions about children’s social development. How does the learn‐preferentially‐from‐the‐high‐in‐status process change as the child grows? Is there a conflict between the evolutionary requirement that children be adapted to their current age and situation if they are to thrive or at least survive, and the need to acquire knowledge and values that will be valuable in adult life (e.g., if the local group laughs at school achievement but skill in STEM [Science, Technology, Engineering and Math] fields is key to future success)? Is some of the sturm und drang of adolescence a formally adaptive switch from perceiving parents as very high in status to perceiving other adolescents or non‐kind adults as very high in status and so learning from these new sources of cultural information? Are different categories of information processed differently at different ages? Do the cultural information editing mechanisms differ when biological sex differs? Do the different types of attachment discussed earlier in this chapter impact the operation of this cultural editing process? Are there distinct editing mechanisms that have not yet been identified? Are the mechanisms affected by family composition, health status, or by climate or subsistence economy?
Researchers in the social development of children have chosen their field wisely: There is no shortage of future research questions.
References
1 Ainsworth, M. D. (1967). Infancy in Uganda. John Hopkins University Press.
2 Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709–716.
3 Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development. American Psychologist, 46(4), 331–341.
4 Auchus, R. J., & Rainey, W. E. (2004). Adrenarche–physiology, biochemistry and human disease. Clinical Endocrinology, 60(3), 288–296.
5 Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture. University of Toronto.
6 Barkow, J. H. (2014). Prestige and the ongoing process of culture revision. In J. T. Cheng, J. L. Tracy & C. Anderson (Eds.), The psychology of social status (pp. 29–46). Springer Science+Business Media.
7 Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press.
8 Barkow, J. H., O’Gorman, R., & Rendell, L. (2012). Are the new mass media subverting cultural transmission? Review of General Psychology 16(2): 121–133.
9 Barresi, M. J., & Gilbert, S. F. (2020). Developmental biology. Sinauer Associates.
10 Belsky, J. (1997). Attachment, mating, and parenting: An evolutionary interpretation. Human Nature, 8(4), 361–381.
11 Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.). Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 139–163). Guilford Press.
12 Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary