Positive Psychology. Группа авторов

Читать онлайн книгу.

Positive Psychology - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
scientific fields, even in the face of some notorious examples of negative creativity – manufactured data, massaging of data, serious misinterpretations of data – need to enhance their emphasis on honest and transparent science, but not at the expense of creativity. But there is a risk that we will educate students in ways that enhance their analytical contributions but not their creative ones. Instead, as a field, scientists should simultaneously increase their emphasis both on honest and transparent analysis but also on positive creativity – doing research that not only is career enhancing, but that is field enhancing and potentially even world enhancing. We might ask ourselves what is the research we can do that will truly make a positive and meaningful difference to our field of endeavor?

      If we don’t always succeed – and we won’t – at least we will have tried. The emphasis in the fields of science, on this view, should shift from creativity – which can be positive but also can be neutral or negative – to positive creativity, which will make our fields better, and that, potentially, will make the world a better place, even if just a tiny little bit. There are many examples, but certainly one example would be that of how to lead people to make better decisions in their lives, both for themselves and for others (Kahneman, 2013; Thaler & Sunstein, 2009).

      The good thing about the new approaches to science is that they may encourage more transparency and honesty. Because of pressures to publish, difficulties in getting large samples, and the mere fact that underpowered studies could get into good journals, we have been less careful than we needed to be in ensuring that our studies are ones that future researchers can rely on. The cost of such carelessness, on all our parts, has been reliance on results that are “will‐o’‐the‐wisps,” setting back the field.

      Developing positive creativity would go beyond what is required for developing creativity that can be positive, neutral, or negative. It would mean additionally asking oneself (a) what are the benefits of and positive uses to which my work can be put? (b) What can I do to augment the positive uses and benefits? (c) What are potential harms of my work? (d) What can I do to mitigate the potential harms? (e) What am I not seeing because I do not want to see it, such as long‐term effects beyond short‐term ones? That is, developing positive creativity would mean developing creativity leavened by intelligence and wisdom (WICS). It would mean thinking about not just coming up with novel and useful ideas, but also what the future implications and uses of these ideas would be. At the same time, we want to ensure the importance of the analytical component – that we assess whether the ideas are truly good ones that work.

      There is no way to guarantee that one’s ideas will be put to positive use. But one greatly can enhance the probability of this happening if only one gives it some thought. I am doubtful that we scientists are teaching students to give their work that kind of thought (Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2004). We can become so preoccupied with career advancement and sometimes short‐range scientific advancement that we may not think about the long term. If we truly want to benefit science as well as education and society, we need to think long term, and we need to foster positive creativity, not merely creativity that may be neutral or negative.

      Being creative is usually uncomfortable and can be potentially dangerous. As noted above, it potentially involves defying the crowd, defying oneself, and defying the zeitgeist (Sternberg, 2018). There is a good reason that people always have been reluctant to be creative. They risk falling prey to the “tall poppy” phenomenon, whereby they end up as the tall poppy in a large field of poppies that gets cut down. The world at large needs positive creativity more than ever before. In our push to be transparent, we need to ensure that we encourage positive creative thinking. And, perhaps most of all, we need to encourage simultaneously not only the best science, but also the careful reflection, courtesy, civility, and plain decency that has come to be lacking in so much of contemporary discourse.

      1 Clark, K., & James, K. (1999). Justice and positive and negative creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 311–320.

      2 Cropley, D. H., Cropley, A. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Runco, M. A. (Eds.). (2010). The dark side of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

      3 Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent creativity: A functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 105–115.

      4 Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., White, A. E., & Chiera, B. A. (2014). Layperson perceptions of malevolent creativity: The good, the bad, and the ambiguous. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 400–412.

      5 Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture, and person: A systems view of creativity. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 325–339). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

      6 Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2013). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.

      7 Falk, W. (2018, February 16). Editor’s letter. The Week, p. 3.

      8 James, K., Clark, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Positive and negative creativity in groups, institutions, and organizations: A model and theoretical extension. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 211–226.

      9 James, K., & Taylor, A. (2010). Positive creativity and negative creativity (and unintended consequences). In D. H. Cropley, A. J. Cropley, J. C. Kaufman, & M. A. Runco (Eds.), The dark side of creativity (pp. 33–56). New York: Cambridge University Press.

      10 Kahneman, D. (2013). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux.

      11 Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The Four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 1–12.

      12 Kaufman, J. C., & Glăveanu, V. (2018). The road to uncreative science is paved with good intentions: Ideas, implementations, and uneasy balances. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(4), 457–465.

      13 Kaufman,


Скачать книгу