The Science of Reading. Группа авторов
Читать онлайн книгу.E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E‐Z Reader model of eye‐movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 445–476. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x03000104.
131 Rueckl, J. G., Paz‐Alonso, P. M., Molfese, P. J., Kuo, W.‐J., Bick, A., Frost, S. J., … Frost, R. (2015). Universal brain signature of proficient reading: Evidence from four contrasting languages. Proceedings National Academy of Sciences, 112, 15510–15515. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509321112.
132 Saygin, Z. M., Osher, D. E., Norton, E. S., Youssoufian, D. A., Beach, S. D., Feather, J., … Kanwisher, N. (2016). Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area. Nature Neuroscience, 19(9), 1250–1255. doi: 10.1038/nn.4354.
133 Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goal, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. doi: 10.4324/9780203781036.
134 Seidenberg, M. S. (2011). Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions. In P. McCardle, J. Ren, O. Tzeng, & B. Miller (Eds.), Dyslexia across languages: Orthography and the brain‐gene‐behavior link (pp. 146–168). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
135 Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523–568. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.96.4.523.
136 Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self‐teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010‐0277(94)00645‐2.
137 Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self‐teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(4), 267–298. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.001.
138 Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584–615. doi: 10.1037/0033‐2909.134.4.584.
139 Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E. Blachman, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Skudlarski, P., … Gore, J. C., (2004). Development of left occipito‐temporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically‐based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55(9), 926–933. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.019.
140 Shu, H., McBride‐Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133. doi: 10.1037/0022‐0663.98.1.122.
141 Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M. Sarkari, S., Billingsley, R. L, Denton, C., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2007). Altering the brain circuits for reading through intervention: A magnetic source imaging study. Neuropsychology, 21(4), 485–496. doi: 10.1037/0894‐4105.21.4.485.
142 Singer, M., Halldorson, M., Lear, J. C., & Andrusiak, P. (1992). Validation of causal bridging inferences in discourse understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 507–524. doi: 10.1016/0749‐596X(92)90026‐T.
143 Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading constrained by culture: Evidence from Chinese. Nature, 431, 71–76. doi: 10.1038/nature02865.
144 Snell, J., van Leipsig, S., Grainger, J., & Meeter, M. (2018). OB1‐reader: A model of word recognition and eye movements in text reading. Psychological Review, 125(6), 969–984. doi: 10.1037/rev0000119.
145 Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358–373. doi: 10.1111/1467‐8624.7402003.
146 Snowling, M. J., Lervåg, A., Nash, H. M., & Hulme, C. (2019). Longitudinal relationships between speech perception, phonological skills and reading in children at high‐risk of dyslexia. Developmental science, 22(1), e12723. doi: 10.1111/desc.12723.
147 Snowling, M. J., Stackhouse, J., & Rack, J. P. (1986). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: A developmental analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 309–339. doi: 10.1080/02643298608253362.
148 Stafura, J. Z., & Perfetti, C. A. (2014). Word‐to‐text integration: Message level and lexical level influences in ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 64, 41–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.012.
149 Stein, N., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse processing: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
150 Strong, G. K., Torgerson, C. J., Torgerson, D., & Hulme, C. (2011). A systematic meta‐analytic review of evidence for the effectiveness of the “Fast ForWord” language intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 224–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐7610.2010.02329.x.
151 Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9(2), 182–198. doi: 10.1016/0093‐934x(80)90139‐x.
152 Tan, L.H., Spinks, J.A., Eden, G., Perfetti, C.A., & Siok, W.T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. PNAS, 102, 8781–8785. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503523102.
153 Taylor, J. S. H., Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H. (2013). Can cognitive models explain brain activation during word and pseudoword reading? A meta‐analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 766–791. doi: 10.1037/a0030266.
154 Trabasso, T, Secco, T., & van den Broek. P. W. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence. In H. Mandl, N. L. Stein, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 83–111). Hillsdale, NJ; Erlbaum.
155 Trabasso, T., & Suh, S. (1993). Understanding text: Achieving explanatory coherence through on‐line references and mental operations in working memory. Discourse Processes, 16(1–2), 3–34. doi: 10.1080/01638539309544827.
156 Turkeltaub, P., Gareau, L., Flowers, D., Zeffiro, T., & Eden, G. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms of reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 767–773. doi: 10.1038/nn1065.
157 van den Broek, P., & Helder, A. (2017). Cognitive processes in discourse comprehension: Passive processes, reader‐initiated processes, and evolving mental representations. Discourse Processes, 54(5–6), 360–372. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2017.1306677.
158 van den Broek, P., Rapp, D. N., & Kendeou, P. (2005). Integrating memory‐based and constructionist approaches in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299–316. doi: 10.1207/s15326950dp3902&3_11.
159 van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye‐Hartmann, E. (1995). The role of readers’ standards for coherence in the generation of inferences during reading. In R. F. Lorch Jr., & E. J. O’Brien (Eds.), Sources of coherence in reading (pp. 353–373). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
160 van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The landscape model of reading. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 71–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
161 van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. Academic Press.
162 van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15, 181–198. doi: 10.3758/bf03197716.
163 van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., & Stone, G. O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97(4), 488–522. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.97.4.488.
164 Vellutino, F. R. (1981). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
165 Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (Eds.). (2017a). Learning to read across languages and writing systems. Cambridge University Press.
166 Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2017b). Operating principles in learning to read. In L. Verhoeven & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.), Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 1–30). Cambridge University Press.
167 Verhoeven, L., Perfetti,